Critical Minds and Critical Time

Jessica George

“The habits we form from childhood make no small difference, but rather they make all the difference” - Aristotle

This opening quote was the perspective that Harvard University’s neuromolecular biologist Dr. Takao Hensch implemented in his talk about manipulating brain plasticity. Children are faster than adults when learning motor outputs, languages, and a plethora of other skills necessary for development. You may know names such as Mozart or Serena Williams; their success in composing and tennis, respectively, was the result of an accumulation of years of experience within these fields. It may not be surprising that people who have achieved great performances often start early in life and acquire the skills that made them extraordinary.  

Brain plasticity, or the brain’s “willingness to change,” is one way to explain a child’s malleability to early experience. Interestingly, this plasticity is different throughout an individual’s lifespan.  For example, let’s look at something that mostly all humans eventually acquire in their skills repertoire: the ability to speak and comprehend language. It’s truly extraordinary to see an infant with near-zero comprehension of language develop a full working vocabulary by the age of four. But what’s truly perplexing is that the same child, when older, struggles significantly more when trying to learn a new language. How can humans be so proficient at acquiring a language with zero past experience during one point of their life, yet so much worse at acquiring new languages later in life -- when they have already developed the neural mechanisms necessary for language?

“What makes us all distinct individuals is this period when environment can act dramatically on the circuits and shape them to become tailor made hardware for the environments into which we are thrown.” - Hensch

This period Hensch refers to is the critical period. Critical periods can be understood as windows of opportunity, in which early experiences can shape brain function for a variety of processes, such as vision and language. During the critical period for language, for example, a child’s exposure to language is extremely influential on the wiring of her brain; thus, she learns at a very efficient rate. However once her critical period closes later in life, her plasticity to experience decreases, and thus she learns languages slower and with more effort. So how does experience translate into changing neural connections? Well, these experiences serve as sensory inputs to two types of neurons in the brain: excitatory neurons and inhibitory neurons. It’s through their communication which makes all the magic happen.

Excitatory neurons drive target cells towards being active and inhibitory neurons prevent target cells from firing action potentials (the method of communication between neurons). Historically, research has been more interested in the implications of excitatory cells because they far outnumber inhibitory cells; the accepted thought was that you need to excite the brain to induce change. However, recent research has indicated that inhibitory circuitry is behind these highly plastic periods of learning.

 Created by Jessica George

Created by Jessica George

There are a few theories that explain why inhibitory neurons are necessary for critical periods. One explanation is that neural inhibition sharpens the signal-to-noise ratio. Think about an outdated TV set. While watching the screen, sometimes you may see a bunch of little dots that don’t contribute to the the clarity of the picture. These dots are the noise, whereas the picture is the signal. We see in newer TV models that these white dots are almost unrecognizable -- indicating that companies have effectively worked to sharpen the signal-to-noise ratio. This analogy can be understood when thinking of inhibitory cells as the TV companies, incoming sensory information as the picture, and noise as all the extra stuff we pick up that our bodies don’t really want to process. Research has shown that immature brains before critical periods have a lot of this “noise”, or spontaneous activity not driven by sensory input. And just like TV companies worked on strengthening this signal-to-noise ratio, our neural system ultimately works to get stronger. Furthermore, the rules of “neurons that fire together wire together” and “neurons out of sync lose their link” are the underlying mechanisms to how some of this noise is lost and signal strength is increased. Adding inhibitory networks allows responses to get sharper and the noise signal to be preferentially dampened.

So what’s the point of understanding how and why critical periods happen?

Interestingly, understanding environmental effects on neural networks can have great implications for society, particularly in education. Preliminary research on neuroplasticity in cats looked at their visual systems and how the environment influences maturation (Frégnac & Imbert, 1978). That is, early environmental disadvantage (such as perpetual darkness) can prevent visual development in cats. This knowledge of environmental influence on visual systems was also found to apply to humans, as well.  Researchers found that when it came to certain visual deficits (ie Amblyopia or lazy eye), the effectiveness of the treatment interventions was highly dependent on the age of the subject (Holmes et al., 2011).  Dr. Allyson Mackey and Dr. Emily Cooper evaluated implications of such findings in their review Sensory and cognitive plasticity: implications for academic interventions. This idea of critical time periods for development, when extended to other neural systems, reveal that other forms of environmental disadvantage (e.g. low SES status, lack of available resources, early childhood stress, etc.) significantly affect cognitive development. However, Dr. Mackey and Dr. Cooper stressed that it’s important to understand that early interventions are not necessarily optimal if they take place before the critical period opens. Moving toward neuroscientific applications within society, understanding trajectories of developmental neural plasticity could be extremely helpful when determining the timing of interventions aiming to ameliorate environmental disadvantages of children in high-risk communities.


About the Author:
Jessica is a junior studying Cognitive Science at the University of Pennsylvania, with a concentration in Cognitive Neuroscience. She is also double-minoring in Psychology and Urban Education: Policy, Research, and Practice. Jessica has volunteered for The Changing Brain Lab since the Fall of 2017. Any questions can be emailed to jege@sas.upenn.edu.

Global Childhood Poverty and Neuroscience Workshops

Anna Meaney

How does socioeconomic status affect brain development, and can neuroscience aid in the effort to alleviate global child poverty?  As Dr. Martha Farah, the director of the Penn Center for Neuroscience & Society, says: “We’re not here because we want more photogenic brains on an MRI.  We don’t really care about their brains as an end in itself.  We care about their lives and the neuroscience is only interesting insofar as it tells us something about that.”  A working group with diverse research backgrounds put on the Global Childhood Poverty and Neuroscience workshops to attempt to shed some light on issues pertaining to brain development, neuroscience, and child poverty. Included in the first workshop was Dr. Martha Farah; Dr. Sebastian Lipina, a visiting academic from the National Council of Research (CONICET) in Argentina; and Dr. Allyson Mackey from Penn Psychology.

Dr. Farah presented research showing the widening gap in academic performance between children of higher and lower socioeconomic status (SES). A 2009 study by Stevens et al. looked at the effect of SES on attention by simultaneously playing two different stories in each participant's ear while asking them to attune to only one of the stories.  Participants with lower parental education attainment had a harder time focusing their attention on one of the stories.  Why does this difference exist? It is possible that this difference stems from higher unpredictability in lower SES households, where it could be more advantageous to have one’s attention spread out and attuned to many different things at once.  However, this finding can also mean that kids from lower SES backgrounds have trouble paying attention to just one thing, which can be a problem in a busy classroom.

Dr. Farah also discussed a Demir et al. paper from 2015 that looked at brain activity correlated with better math performance.  It was found that better math performance was correlated with activity in verbal regions of the brain in children from higher SES backgrounds.  However, for children from lower SES backgrounds, visual and spatial areas of the brain were correlated with better math performance.  This finding provided evidence that SES is associated with the way the brain processes information, which can have numerous practical implications about the way math can best be taught to kids from lower SES backgrounds, such as demonstrating math visually rather than verbally describing how something works.  Together, these studies look beyond just gaps in test scores and provide evidence for specific learning differences in children from varying SES backgrounds.

Dr. Mackey corroborated Dr. Farah's presentation by discussing how children from a lower SES background are more likely to be exposed to a number of risk factors that impact brain development, and how low SES exacerbates those risks.These include physical risks (e.g. pesticides, lead) as well as physiological risks (e.g. sleep disruption, lack of exercise, poor medical care, malnutrition). Additionally, there are psycho-social elements such as discrimination, eviction, crime, incarceration, unstable families, addiction, and low quality schools.  All of these factors are stressors on child brain development.

For the workshop, Dr. Mackey particularly discussed how stress and sleep deprivation can lead to changes the brain.

Evidence suggests that early life stress alters the reward system in mice (Peña et al. 2017). Mice who were stressed in early life and then encountered social stress in adulthood developed depression-like behaviors (Peña et al. 2017).  While mice and humans are different, children who have experienced more stressful life events have weakened connectivity between certain areas of the brain, potentially indicating a weakening level of neuroplasticity in children, which can make it increasingly difficult to learn new things (Park et al in prep).

Sleep is another domain in which low SES can exacerbate risk factors associated with brain development. Children from lower SES households are at a higher risk for poor sleep quality compared to children from higher SES households (Buckhalt et al 2011).  For kids from lower SES, sleep loss is associated with a great reduction in cognitive performance.  Sleep disturbances are also associated with reduced whole brain volume and dorsolateral prefrontal thickness, an area that is important in complex problem solving and decision making. (Kocevska et al 2016).

However, for children from higher SES who do not see the same reduction as those who experience sleep loss and are of lower SES, this is not the case.  Parents who are concerned about how sleep quality might be affecting their child’s brain development can track their child’s sleep to help determine if there are poor sleep conditions at home.  There are also specific steps that can be taken for parents who want their children to sleep better, such as making sure the child has a consistent bedtime and limiting media use around bedtime.  

Despite all of this research that link SES and brain development, the path from research to policy is a long one.  This reality was demonstrated to the audience with the example of the state’s car seat policy - it took 10 years for Pennsylvania to change the policy regarding car seats for children under 2 years old after a study was published saying children under the age of 2 are 75% less likely to die or be severely injured in a crash if they are sitting rear-facing.

Because it takes so long for science to influence policy, it is important to think about the small steps parents and educators can take now.  Neuroscience can have real world effects that can make a difference in children’s lives, and fortunately, the field is filled with scientists who are committed to thinking about the real-world implications of their research.


About the Author:
Anna is a senior at Vassar College where she studies Neuroscience with a focus on evolution.  She volunteered in The Changing Brain lab this summer.  Anna can be reached at anmeaney@vassar.edu with any questions.   

 

 

The Effect of Effort on Test Scores

Connor Kendzora

Little research has been conducted to assess students’ performance in low-stakes testing situations in comparison to high-stakes situations. Low-stakes tests do not heavily impact the test-taker’s grade in a class or the course of their academic career. On the contrary, high-stakes tests usually do impact the test-taker’s grade or academic career.

Previous studies have only made statistical comparisons using a between-subjects design, meaning that a different sample of students was used for each condition (high-stakes and low-stakes). This leads to more individual differences between groups and, consequently, less statistical power. A comparison using a repeated-measures design, in which the same sample of participants is used for each condition, could be valuable, as it may reveal that students’ performance on low-stakes tests does not actually match the student's’ proficiency in the tested subject, as a result of a deficiency in effort compared to high stakes testing.

Yigal Attali (2016), influenced by past studies that revealed a negative difference in low-stakes scores from high-stakes scores, performed such a comparison, where he used data from an existing study of the effect of extra time on GRE scores by Bridgeman, Cline and Hessinger (2004) in order to determine the effect of effort in low stakes assessments.

Attali measured the participants’ operational scores (the score received on the actual GRE), research scores (score received on the research section, or the extra section of the GRE the participants had been asked to complete), and the amount of effort exerted by each participant during the research section (time taken to complete). The author justified his definition of effort as the amount of time taken to complete the research section by citing that self-report measures are often biased as subjects are often reluctant to admit they did not try their hardest. He argues that the longer a test-taker spends on a test, the fewer short, rapid-guess responses they will give, indicating a larger amount of effort.

Results of statistical analyses showed that the average research score was considerably lower than the average operational score. This was true for both the verbal and quantitative sections. Average time spent on the research section was only 63% of the time spent on the operational section. It was also found that 2% of the research group scored the lowest possible score on their section, compared to only 0.2% in the operational group. This indicates that many test-takers did not perform to their potential by giving less effort on the research section. Attali also checked for effects of motivational filtering, or filtering out scores of students who clearly gave minimal effort on the research section. He accomplished this by examining what the data would look like if minimal effort attempts were removed at increasing intervals. Removing the bottom 18% (in terms of time taken to complete the research section) of test takers greatly decreased the effect of the stakes and increased the correlation between the research scores and operational scores.

In summary, low-stakes test scores and high-stakes test scores can have a strong correlation when filtering out the test-takers who give effort unrepresentative of their ability. This is something to be kept in mind by organizations who seek to draw conclusions about students’ proficiency from low-stakes tests.


About the Author:  
Connor is a Psychology major and incoming senior at California State University, Long Beach. He worked in The Changing Brain Lab during Summer 2017 and currently works as an ABA therapist. Any questions can be emailed to connorkendzora@gmail.com.

Welcome!

Hello!
The members of The Changing Brain Lab would like to officially introduce our new blog! We will use this space in order to discuss new research that pertains to our interests, events on campus or in the community that we are involved with, and news regarding our lab. We will be releasing a blog post approximately once every month, so please check back whenever possible to see what our lab has been up to! Information regarding the author of each piece will follow each published post. If you have any questions or comments regarding the papers or events discussed, please feel free to contact our lab at mackeylab@sas.upenn.edu or via phone at (215) 573-1670.